Skip to main content

In-utero exposure to phenols and phthalates and the intelligence quotient of boys at 5 years

Abstract

Background

There are concerns that developmental exposure to endocrine disrupting chemicals such as phenolic compounds and phthalates could affect child cognitive function. Epidemiological studies tackling this question have mainly focused on phthalate metabolites and bisphenol A, but not on the other phenolic compounds. Our study aimed to assess the relationship between in-utero exposure to phthalates, bisphenol A and other phenolic compounds (parabens, triclosan, dichlorophenols and benzophenone-3) and the Intelligence Quotient (IQ) of boys at 5–6 years.

Methods

In 452 mother-son dyads from the French EDEN cohort, we measured 11 phthalate metabolites and 9 phenolic compounds (4 parabens, benzophenone-3, bisphenol A, 2 dichlorophenols and triclosan) in spot urine samples collected between 22 and 29 gestational weeks. Verbal and performance IQ of children were assessed at 5–6 years by a psychologist using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI). We used adjusted Structural Equation Models (SEM) combined with Benjamini and Hochberg false discovery rate correction to assess the associations between maternal urine phenol and phthalate metabolite concentrations considered simultaneously and the boys’ IQ.

Results

No phenol or phthalate metabolite concentration was negatively associated with the boys’ verbal or performance IQ (uncorrected p-values ≥0.09). Mono(3-carboxypropyl) phthalate tended to be associated with increased verbal IQ (β = 0.136, 95% confidence interval, 0.01; 0.27). This association disappeared after correction for multiple comparison (corrected p-value, 0.71).

Conclusion

Our results did not provide evidence of an inverse association between in-utero exposure to phenols or phthalates and verbal and performance IQ among boys. Since phenols and phthalates may have sex-specific effects, these null findings cannot be generalized to girls. Limitations included use of a single spot urine sample to assess exposures and lack of consideration of postnatal exposures.

Peer Review reports

Background

Phenols and phthalates are synthetic compounds with wide application including, manufacture of polycarbonate plastics (bisphenol A), UV-filters in sun screens (benzophenone-3), microbicides in toothpaste and soaps (triclosan), preservatives in food, cosmetics and personal care products (parabens), plasticizers in food packing material, personal care products, medications and residential floor materials (phthalates). Exposure to phenols and phthalates is ubiquitous in the general population and these compounds have been detected in urine samples of mothers and their children [1], amniotic fluid [2, 3] and umbilical cord blood [4]. Some phenols and phthalates have endocrine disrupting capabilities [5,6,7] and can indeed interfere with normal synthesis, metabolism, secretion, transport, receptor binding, action, or elimination of endogenous hormones, including the thyroid and sex hormones [8,9,10,11] that are involved in fetal brain development [12, 13]. Disruption of the normal process of brain development can lead to impaired or abnormal development of cognitive skills [14]. Functionally, cognitive development can be classified in several interrelated domains such as intelligence, attention, language and communication, executive function, visuospatial abilities and learning and memory [15]. In this study, we assessed effects on the intelligence quotient (IQ), a psychometric approach that assesses the child’s ability to complete verbal and performance tasks.

Several publications have explored the associations between prenatal exposure to phenols and behavior [6, 16], a social-emotional domain that encompasses developmental adaptivity of a child to its social environment and not under the scope of this paper. Only five studies, majorly focusing on bisphenol A, have examined child cognitive development in relation to urinary [17,18,19,20] and umbilical cord blood [21] bisphenol A concentrations. Among these, three assessed child IQ and neither found any association with maternal urinary bisphenol A concentrations except one suggesting a negative association with IQ and bisphenol A measured in cord blood [21]. To our knowledge, no study has examined IQ in relation to prenatal exposure to other phenols such as triclosan, dichlorophenols, parabens or benzophenone-3.

Regarding phthalates, epidemiological studies on the cognitive function mainly assessed associations with the mental development index of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID) or with IQ of children between 6 months and 11 years of age. With two exceptions reporting null associations in relation to IQ [22, 23], most of these studies reported negative associations between maternal urinary concentrations of several phthalate metabolites with the mental development index at 6 months [24], 2 years [25, 26] and 3 years [27] and full scale IQ of children at 7 [28]. Depending on the study, some of the phthalates incriminated were mono-n-butyl phthalate (MBP) [24, 26, 28], di(2-ethylhexyl) phthalate (DEHP) metabolites [24, 26], mono-isobutyl phthalate (MIBP) [28], monobenzyl phthalate (MBZP) [28] and mono(3-carboxypropyl) phthalate (MCPP) [25].

In summary, previous human literature did not report clear association between prenatal exposure to bisphenol A and child IQ, while negative associations have been reported by most studies for phthalates, at various ages. We were not able to identify epidemiological studies that explored associations between phenols other than bisphenol A and child cognition.

In this study, we examined associations between maternal urinary phenol and phthalate metabolite concentrations and IQ of boys at 5–6 years with the aim of replicating deleterious effects between phthalate and the child cognitive function observed in previous studies [24,25,26,27,28] and providing exploratory results regarding the potential effects of prenatal exposure to phenols such as triclosan and parabens on child cognitive function. With regards to previous literature in humans that did not report clear association between prenatal exposure to bisphenol A and cognitive function [17,18,19,20], we were not expecting an association with child IQ for this chemical.

Methods

Study population

The study population was a sample of mother-son pairs of the EDEN (Etude des Déterminants pré et post natals du développement et de la santé de l’Enfant) cohort [29]. The EDEN cohort consists of 2002 pregnant women recruited before the end of the 28th gestational week from February 2003 through January 2006 in the obstetric departments of Nancy and Poitiers university hospitals, France. The cohort received approval from the ethics committee of Kremlin-Bicêtre University hospital. To be included, participants gave informed written consent for themselves and for their children.

Among the 998 boys of the EDEN cohort born alive, 452 had IQ assessments at 5 years and urinary concentrations of phenols and phthalate metabolites assessed in maternal urine. Urinary concentrations of phenols and phthalate metabolites were assessed in a framework of previous projects that evaluated associations between these compounds, male genital malformation [30] and growth [31,32,33], explaining why we focused on boys. Inclusion criteria for biomarker assessments was being a boy, having at least one urine sample available during pregnancy and having data on growth during the pre and postnatal period (up to 3 years). While this prevents generalizability of our findings to females, focusing on one sex is not a source of bias. Moreover, in the context where sex specific effects are plausible [17, 24, 25, 27], a study restricted to one sex will indeed have higher statistical power than a study of similar sample size including both sexes in which two sex-specific analyses are conducted.

Assessment of IQ

IQ was assessed at an average age of 5.7 (Standard Deviation (SD): 0.1) years using the French version of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Third Edition (WPPSI-III) [34]. This scale was translated, culturally adapted to French norms, and calibrated in a sample of 999 children representative of the French children between aged 2.5 and 7.25 years by the ECPA (Editions of the Centre in Applied Psychology) [35]. Assessments were carried out by two trained psychologists using the seven core subtests to compute the verbal and performance IQ scores. The seven subtests include: information (number of correct answers to questions that address a broad range of general knowledge topics, 34 items), vocabulary (number of words correctly defined, 25 items), word reasoning (number of concepts correctly identified from a series of clues, 28 items), block design (number of correct designs recreated using blocks, 20 items), matrix reasoning (number of matrices correctly completed, 29 items), picture concepts subtest (number of correct selections of 2 or 3 pictures with common characteristics, 28 items) and coding (number of correct symbols copied corresponding to geometric shapes, 59 items) [36].

Exposure assessment

We measured concentrations of bisphenol A, benzophenone-3, triclosan, 2,4 and 2,5 dichlorophenol, methyl-, ethyl-, propyl- and butyl-paraben along with 11 phthalate metabolites from maternal urine samples collected during pregnancy between 22 and 29 gestational weeks using online solid phase extraction-high performance liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry [37, 38]. Creatinine, a proxy of urine dilution, was also measured. Assessments were performed at the National Center for Environmental Health laboratory at the CDC in Atlanta, Georgia, USA.

For concentrations below the limit of detection we used the instrumental reading values. To allow ln-transformation, instrumental reading values equal to 0 (i.e. indicative of no signal) were replaced by the lowest instrumental reading value divided by the square root of two [32]. To limit the impact of between subject variations in urine sampling conditions, concentrations were standardized for collection conditions such as hour of sampling, gestational age at collection, duration of storage at room temperature before freezing, day of sampling, year of biomarker assessments at CDC (2008 or 2011) and creatinine levels. This was done using a 2 steps statistical approach [32, 39]. We first studied the influence of each sampling condition and creatinine levels on the measured biomarker concentrations using adjusted linear regression. We then predicted standardized concentrations using the effect estimates for each sampling condition that was associated with the biomarker concentrations (p-value < 0.2) and the measured biomarker concentrations [39]. The standardized concentrations assumed that all samples were collected under similar conditions and were used in all of our analyses.

Confounding

Confounders were identified a priori from literature and included variables associated with both exposure and the outcome, without being likely consequences of the exposures thereof, associated with the outcomes only. These included center of recruitment (Poitiers/Nancy), maternal age (continuous), parity (nulliparous/≥1 child), maternal body mass index (BMI, continuous), parental education level (average number of years spent in school by mother and father), breastfeeding duration (never/breastfed), monthly household income (≤1500, 1500 – 3000 and ≥3000 Euros), smoking during pregnancy (yes/no), maternal psychological difficulties during pregnancy (yes/no), child cognitive stimulation (continuous) and finally, child age at assessment (continuous). In our main analysis, we used crude IQ subscale scores and adjusted for child age because we observed negative associations (results not shown) between the age standardized scores with child age at IQ assessment. Child cognitive stimulation was assessed using items from the short form of the Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment Scale questionnaire (HOME) at 5 years. The maternal psychological difficulties score during pregnancy was constructed by combining scores of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale Revised (CESD) and of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) two questionnaires designed to assess depression and anxiety respectively. Among the 452 mother-son pairs included in the study, several covariates had missing values. We used multiple imputation using the mi impute chained command in STATA14 with 5 imputed datasets [40] to handle the missing data for the HOME score (N = 25, 5.5%), BMI (N = 4, 0.9%), parity, income and maternal depression and psychological difficulties (N = 1, 0.2%).

Association between biomarker concentrations and the boys’ IQ

All biomarker concentrations were ln-transformed before analysis. We used Structural Equation Models (SEM) to study the associations between the urinary biomarker concentrations and the boys’ IQ scores. SEM simultaneously assesses relationships between one or more independent variables and one or more dependent variables. They comprise of two parts, 1) a measurement model based on factor analysis that relates observed variables to latent constructs and 2) a structural model, based on ordinary regression which describes the relationships between the generated latent exposure and outcome constructs. Latent constructs underlying the IQ outcomes included 2 factors: the verbal and performance factors, that were determined a priori from previous literature [41, 42]. The Confirmatory Factor Analysis we performed on our data also supported this two-factor structure (Additional file 1: Table S1). The performance IQ factor was constructed using the matrix reasoning, block design and picture concept scores while verbal IQ factor was constructed using the information, vocabulary and word reasoning scores of the WPPSI (Fig. 1). For the exposures, we first considered 3 latent variables based on factorial analysis: one predicted by the 2 dichlorophenols, one by the 4 parabens and one by the 4 DEHP metabolites. Because the latent variable based on the two dichlorophenols caused the model to be unidentified, we decided not to include a latent variable for the dichlorophenols and instead included only one of the dichlorophenols (2,5 dichlorophenol) as an observed variable. The final model had 2 latent exposure variables representing paraben and DEHP exposures and all other biomarker concentrations were added as observed variables (Fig. 1). We allowed the error covariance between, methyl and propyl paraben, butyl and ethyl paraben and the latent variable of parabens with monoethyl phthalate to be freely estimated. Estimating covariance between the DEHP metabolites freely did not improve the fit of the model and was not included in our final model. We reported standardized association estimates, expressed as the change in SD of IQ scores associated with a 1-SD increase in the ln-transformed biomarker concentrations.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Path diagram illustrating associations between phenols, phthalate metabolites and IQ of boys. Shapes representations: circles: latent variables, rectangles: observed variables, dashed and solid arrows: linear relationships, double headed arrows: correlations. Abbreviations: MP: methyl paraben, EP: ethyl paraben, PP: propyl paraben, BP: butyl paraben, DEHP: di(2-ethylhexyl) phthalate metabolites, MECPP: mono(2-ethyl-5-carboxypentyl) phthalate, MEHHP: mono(2-ethyl-5-hydroxyhexyl) phthalate, MEOHP: mono(2-ethyl-5-oxohexyl) phthalate, MEHP: mono(2-ethylhexyl) phthalate. Phenols represents 2,5-dichlorophenol, benzophenone 3, bisphenol A and triclosan. Phthalates represents mono-n-butyl phthalate, monobenzyl phthalate, monocarboxy-isononyl phthalate, monocarboxy-isooctyl phthalate, mono(3-carboxypropyl) phthalate, monoethyl phthalate and mono-isobutyl phthalate. Confounders include maternal age, age of child at IQ assessment, HOME stimulation score, maternal body mass index before pregnancy, parental education level, parity, monthly revenue, breast feeding duration and maternal psychological difficulties during pregnancy

We tested 26 associations (2 outcomes * 13 biomarkers) and cannot exclude that part of the associations observed were due to chance. For this reason, in addition to the regular p-value we presented in the results section the p-values corrected for multiple testing using the Benjamini and Hochberg false discovery rate (FDR) method [43].

Sensitivity analysis

In the sensitivity analysis, we ran a SEM model using only one construct for IQ (full scale IQ) constructed using all seven subscales of the WPPSI-III (Additional file 1: Figure S1) We also performed analysis using only complete cases (no imputation of the missing covariates). Because exposure levels and IQ scores differed by recruitment centers, we performed additional analysis stratified by center. While commonly used in psychology and sociology, SEM have so far had limited use in environmental epidemiology [44, 45]. For this reason, we also conducted more classic analyses using the manual based IQ scores instead of latent constructs and run linear regression models (one model per outcome and exposure). We explored the shape of the associations between phenols, phthalates and IQ by studying exposures categorized as tertiles.

Analysis was carried out using MPLUS 6 (Muthen & Muthen, Los Angeles, CA, USA) and STATA/SE 14 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA) statistical softwares.

Results

Population characteristics

The majority of mothers in the study population (63%) were recruited from Poitiers. On average, mothers were 29.9 (SD: 4.7) years old and 46% were nulliparous. Fifty-five percent of the mothers and 44% of the fathers had more than 2 years of education after high school. Twenty-eight percent of the mothers reported no breast-feeding at all and about 23% smoked during pregnancy (Table 1). Compared to mother son-pairs of the EDEN cohort not included in our analysis, pairs in our study population were more likely to be from Poitiers, from households that earned > 1500 euros/month, and less likely to have smoked during pregnancy (Table 1). Associations between maternal characteristics and child IQ are shown in Additional file 1: Table S2.

Table 1 Characteristics of the study population and the mother-son pairs of the French EDEN cohort

Most of the compounds assessed were detected in more than 93% of the urine samples except for triclosan, ethyl and butyl paraben that were detected in 73 to 84% of the samples (Table 2). As expected, correlations between the 4 parabens (spearman’s coefficients; 0.46 to 0.82), the 2 dichlorophenols (spearman’s coefficients: 0.69) and the DEHP metabolites (spearman’s coefficients; 0.78 to 0.98) were high. The other coefficients of correlation were in the − 0.11 to 0.61 range (Additional file 1: Table S3).

Table 2 Concentrationsa of phenols and phthalate metabolites in maternal urine (N = 452b mother-son pairs of the EDEN mother child cohort)

Associations between biomarker concentrations and boys’ IQ scores

After adjustment and imputation of the missing covariates, SEM with two latent constructs underlying the IQ outcomes converged normally, and fit indices suggested a statistically good fit: root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) was 0.048, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) was 0.930, Tucker-Lewis fit index (TLI) was 0.921. The standardized factor loadings for indicators of both outcomes and exposure latent factors were uniformly high (standardized factor loadings > 0.53) and significant (p-values < 0.001), suggesting convergent validity [46] (Additional file 1: Table S1).

The only significant (uncorrected p-value below 0.05) association we observed was a positive association between MCPP and verbal IQ (β = 0.136 SD of IQ scores for an 1-SD increase in the ln-transformed MCPP concentration; 95% CI: 0.006; 0.266, Table 3). This association did not remain significant after correction for multiple comparison (corrected p-value = 0.71). No other phenol or phthalate was associated with performance or verbal IQ (uncorrected p-values ≥0.09, Table 3).

Table 3 Adjusted associations between phenols, phthalate metabolites and IQ of boys at 5 years (N = 452)

In the sensitivity analyses, running a SEM model with only one construct for IQ led to similar results and the only significant (uncorrected p-value below 0.05) association observed was a positive association between MCPP and Full Scale IQ (Additional file 1: Table S4).

When we restricted our analysis to mother-child pairs with no missing values on the covariates (N = 419), the positive association observed between MCPP and verbal IQ was slightly attenuated (Table 4): β was 0.109 (95% CI: −0.024; 0.243) compared to 0.136 (95% CI: 0.006; 0.266) in our main analysis and a positive association between triclosan and verbal IQ emerged (β = 0.101, 95% CI: 0.008; 0.194 p-value = 0.034).

Table 4 Adjusted associations between phenols, phthalate metabolites and IQ of boys at 5 years in the EDEN cohort (N = 419 mother-son pairs, complete case analysis

Using the manual based IQ scores and running multivariate linear regression models (one model per outcome and exposure) instead of SEM suggested no significant association between phenols, phthalate metabolites and the IQ scales (all p-values were above 0.13; Additional file 1: Table S5). Similarly, no significant association was observed after categorizing exposure in tertiles (p-value for heterogeneity > 0.07, Additional file 1: Table S6), except for triclosan (p-value for heterogeneity = 0.05), for which results were suggestive of a positive association with verbal IQ. Verbal IQ increased by 3,82, points (95% CI: 0.70; 6.93) and 2.31 points (95% CI: −0.79; 5.46) in the second and third triclosan tertile concentrations respectively (Additional file 1: Table S6).

In stratified analyses, we observed some heterogeneity in associations of IQ with benzophenone-3 and triclosan urinary concentation by center (Additional file 1: Table S7).

Discussion

Results of our main analysis were suggestive of a positive association between maternal urinary concentrations of MCPP and verbal IQ of boys at 5–6 years that did not remain significant after correction for multiple comparisons. No other phenol or phthalate metabolite was associated with the boys’ IQ in our main analysis.

Phthalates and the boys’ IQ

MCPP is a metabolite of di-n-octyl phthalate, used as a plasticizer in the production of plastics to improve flexibility. It is found in products such as carpet back coating, wire, cables, and adhesives [47]. Among the few studies that explored the associations between prenatal exposure to phthalates and the cognitive function, only three examined child IQ [22, 23, 28] and none measured MCPP in urine, limiting comparison. Two of these studies found no significant associations between child IQ at 6 years (N = 182) [23] or 11 years (N = 110) [22] and maternal phthalate metabolite urinary concentrations while they reported negative associations with childhood exposure to DEHP metabolites and MBP. In another study, Factor-Litvak et al., found that when boys and girls were studied altogether, MBP, MBZP and MIBP maternal urinary concentrations were inversely associated with the full-scale IQ at 7 years. After stratifying for sex, Factor-Litvak et al., reported an inverse association between MIBP and the full-scale IQ of boys at 7 years (N = 155) [28]. This study included Hispanics and African Americans women of inner city New-York which could limit comparability with our results that relied on a population that mostly included highly educated Caucasian European women. All three aforementioned studies had smaller population sizes than our study (N = 452 boys): 110 boys and girls [22], 182 boys and girls [23] and 155 boys [28] respectively. While we used IQ to evaluate the cognitive function of children, other assessments such as the Bailey Scales of Infant Development (BSID) could be used. Among studies relying on the BSID, two reported that DEHP metabolites and MEP were negatively associated with the mental index of boys at 6 months and 2 years [24, 26] while another reported positive associations between MCPP along with metabolites of di(n-butyl) phthalate (MBP and MBZP) and this mental index in boys at 2 years [25]. Reports of null associations also exist [48].

As observed in our main analysis, the positive association between MCPP and verbal IQ was not significant after correction for multiple comparisons and the effect direction was opposite to what was reported for other phthalate metabolites in a previous study on IQ [28]. Altogether, these findings did not provide strong support for an effect of MCPP on child IQ.

Phenols and the boys’ IQ

In our study population, prenatal exposure to bisphenol A was not associated with boy’s IQ at 5 years. In line with our results, two epidemiological studies did not report associations between maternal urinary concentrations of bisphenol A and child IQ at 5 and 8 years [19, 20]. Lin et al., reported decreased Full Scale and verbal IQ scores at 7 years in association with higher levels of bisphenol A in cord blood [21]. Frequency of detection of bisphenol A in cord blood was low (56%) compared to levels detected in maternal urine (frequency of detection ranged from 86 to 99% in our study and in two previous studies by Stacy et al. and Braun et al. [19, 20]). Two other studies performed among younger children (1, 3 and 4 years versus 5–6 years in our study) and relying on the BSID, the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA) or the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool (BRIEF-P) to assess cognitive function did not report any association with prenatal exposure to bisphenol A [17] among boys but among girls, who had lower scores on the emotional control and inhibition scale of the BRIEF-P with increased prenatal exposure to bisphenol A [17].

None of the other phenols assessed was associated with IQ of boys in our main analysis, while after stratification for recruitment centers, benzophenone-3 and triclosan were associated with some IQ scores, the direction of the association varying according to IQ scale (performance versus verbal IQ) and recruitment centers. In humans, we did not identify any prior studies assessing the associations of prenatal exposure to dichlorophenol, benzophenone-3, triclosan and parabens with child cognition. In the animal literature, one rodent study attempted to assess the effects of butyl paraben on spatial memory of rats. This study reported that exposure of expectant rats to high levels of butyl paraben (200 mg/kg/day) negatively affected spatial memory in offspring [49]. Although Ali et al. suggest deleterious effects on spatial memory of rats, we did not observe such effects with the IQ of boys at 5–6 years.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of our study included the larger sample size (N = 452 boys) compared to previous studies that examined prenatal exposure to bisphenol A and phthalates and child cognitive development among (N ranged between 64 to 266 boys [17, 19, 20, 22, 24, 26, 28]). We relied on SEM to study the association with cognitive function. SEM permitted grouping of compound esters (parabens), metabolites from similar parent compounds (DEHP metabolites) and IQ outcomes through latent variables. Use of latent variables limited the number of associations tested compared to the classical approaches that relied on the use of one regression model per outcome and exposure. Furthermore, we used the Benjamini and Hochberg FDR method to control for multiple testing and limit chance finding. One limitation of the FDR approach is that it does not take into account correlation across exposures and outcomes variables. Also, the FDR correction approach ignores that the tested compounds are not a random sample of all existing chemicals, but have, for most of them, some a priori plausibility for an effect on neurodevelopment based on the toxicological or epidemiological literature.

As most of the previous studies, we used one single urine measurement to assess exposure. For compounds with a short half-life such as phenols and phthalates, this is known to lead to exposure misclassification. The resulting measurement error is likely to be of classical type and to lead to attenuation bias in the effect estimate and to power loss, which may explain our null findings. In addition, non-classical error may arise in case we did not target the relevant exposure window. We only assessed exposure for boys. Although this is not a source of bias, it impedes drawing conclusions for female offspring. A previous study in girls has reported deleterious effect of bisphenol A on the executive function [17]. We only considered in-utero exposure, while previous studies that assessed both pre and post-natal exposure have suggested adverse IQ associations with phthalate and bisphenol A exposures during childhood and early adolescence [19, 22, 23]. We adjusted for many potential confounders, but residual confounding cannot be ruled out, for example; we did not adjust for nutritional habits and maternal IQ, which could affect either the exposure and/or IQ of the children. We only assessed exposure to phenols and phthalates, while other chemicals might affect child neurodevelopment [50]. A previous study that assessed exposure to 81 environmental factors among Spanish women did not report strong correlation for bisphenol A with any of the remaining 80 exposures, while phthalates were negatively correlated with some metals such as cadmium and copper (rho ranged between − 0.3 and − 0.5) [51], suggesting that associations of bisphenol A and phthalate with health outcomes are unlikely to be confounded by co-exposures.

Conclusion

While we relied on a larger sample size (N = 452 boys) than in previous studies, our study did not provide evidence of an association between any of the phenol and phthalate biomarkers assessed in maternal urine and boy’s IQ at 5–6 years. The use of a spot sample to assess exposure may have limited our statistical power. Since phenols and phthalates may have sex-specific effects, our null findings among boys cannot be generalized to girls. Future studies should consider use of multiple urine samples per individual to assess exposure during pre and post-natal life among boys and girls.

Abbreviations

CESD:

Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale

CI:

Confidence interval

DEHP:

Di(2-ethylhexyl) phthalate

FDR:

False discovery rate

IQ:

Intelligence quotient

LOD:

Limit of detection

MBP:

Mono-n-butyl phthalate

MBZP:

Monobenzyl phthalate

MCNP:

Monocarboxy-isononyl phthalate

MCOP:

Monocarboxy-isooctyl phthalate

MCPP:

Mono(3-carboxypropyl) phthalate

MECPP:

Mono(2-ethyl-5-carboxypentyl) phthalate

MEHHP:

Mono(2-ethyl-5-hydroxyhexyl) phthalate

MEHP:

Mono(2-ethylhexyl) phthalate

MEOHP:

Mono(2-ethyl-5-oxohexyl) phthalate

MEP:

Monoethyl phthalate

MIBP:

Mono-isobutyl phthalate

RMSEA:

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation

SD:

Standard deviation

SEM:

Structural Equation Model

STAI:

The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

WPPSI:

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence

References

  1. Casas M, Chevrier C, Den Hond E, Fernandez MF, Pierik F, Philippat C, et al. Exposure to brominated flame retardants, perfluorinated compounds, phthalates and phenols in European birth cohorts: ENRIECO evaluation, first human biomonitoring results, and recommendations. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2013;216:230–42.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Philippat C, Wolff MS, Calafat AM, Ye X, Bausell R, Meadows M, et al. Prenatal exposure to environmental phenols: concentrations in amniotic fluid and variability in urinary concentrations during pregnancy. Environ Health Perspect. 2013;121:1225–31.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Jensen MS, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Toft G, Hougaard DM, Bonde JP, Cohen A, et al. Phthalates and perfluorooctanesulfonic acid in human amniotic fluid: temporal trends and timing of amniocentesis in pregnancy. Environ Health Perspect National Institute of Environmental Health Science. 2012;120:897–903.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Chou W-C, Chen J-L, Lin C-F, Chen Y-C, Shih F-C, Chuang C-Y. Biomonitoring of bisphenol A concentrations in maternal and umbilical cord blood in regard to birth outcomes and adipokine expression: a birth cohort study in Taiwan. Environ Health. 2011;10:94.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Miodovnik A, Edwards A, Bellinger DC, Hauser R. Developmental neurotoxicity of ortho-phthalate diesters: review of human and experimental evidence. Neurotoxicology. 2014;41:112–22.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Mustieles V, Pérez-Lobato R, Olea N, Fernández MF. Bisphenol A: human exposure and neurobehavior. Neurotoxicology. 2015;49:174–84.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Golden R, Gandy J, Vollmer G. A review of the endocrine activity of parabens and implications for potential risks to human health. Crit Rev Toxicol. 2005;35:435–58.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Boberg J, Taxvig C, Christiansen S, Hass U. Possible endocrine disrupting effects of parabens and their metabolites. Reprod Toxicol. 2010;30:301–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Krause M, Klit A, Blomberg Jensen M, Søeborg T, Frederiksen H, Schlumpf M, et al. Sunscreens: are they beneficial for health? An overview of endocrine disrupting properties of UV-filters. Int J Androl. 2012;35:424–36.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Paul KB, Hedge JM, Bansal R, Zoeller RT, Peter R, DeVito MJ, et al. Developmental triclosan exposure decreases maternal, fetal, and early neonatal thyroxine: a dynamic and kinetic evaluation of a putative mode-of-action. Toxicology. 2012;300:31–45.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Howdeshell KL, Wilson VS, Furr J, Lambright CR, Rider CV, Blystone CR, et al. A mixture of five phthalate esters inhibits fetal testicular testosterone production in the Sprague-Dawley rat in a cumulative, dose-additive manner. Toxicol Sci. 2008;105:153–65.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Colborn T. Neurodevelopment and endocrine disruption. Environ Health Perspect National Institute of Environmental Health Science. 2004;112:944–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Cohen-Bendahan CCC, van de Beek C, Berenbaum SA. Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: methods and findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:353–84.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Rice D, Barone S. Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: evidence from humans and animal models. Environ Health Perspect. 2000;108(Suppl):511–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Forns J, Aranbarri A, Grellier J, Julvez J, Vrijheid M, Sunyer J. A conceptual framework in the study of neuropsychological development in epidemiological studies. Neuroepidemiology. Karger Publishers. 2012;38:203–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Philippat C, Nakiwala D, Calafat AM, Botton J, De Agostini M, Heude B, et al. Prenatal exposure to nonpersistent endocrine disruptors and behavior in boys at 3 and 5 years. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125(9):097014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Braun JM, Kalkbrenner AE, Calafat AM, Yolton K, Ye X, Dietrich KN, et al. Impact of early-life bisphenol A exposure on behavior and executive function in children. Pediatrics. 2011;128:873–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Casas M, Forns J, Martínez D, Avella-García C, Valvi D, Ballesteros-Gómez A, et al. Exposure to bisphenol A during pregnancy and child neuropsychological development in the INMA-Sabadell cohort. Environ Res. 2015;142:671–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Stacy SL, Papandonatos GD, Calafat AM, Chen A, Yolton K, Lanphear BP, et al. Early life bisphenol A exposure and neurobehavior at 8 years of age: identifying windows of heightened vulnerability. Environ Int. 2017;107:258–65.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Braun JM, Muckle G, Arbuckle T, Bouchard MF, Fraser WD, Ouellet E, et al. Associations of prenatal urinary bisphenol A concentrations with child behaviors and cognitive abilities. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125:67008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Lin C-C, Chien C-J, Tsai M-S, Hsieh C-J, Hsieh W-S, Chen P-C. Prenatal phenolic compounds exposure and neurobehavioral development at 2 and 7 years of age. Sci Total Environ. 2017;605–606:801–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Huang H-B, Chen H-Y, Su P-H, Huang P-C, Sun C-W, Wang C-J, et al. Fetal and childhood exposure to phthalate diesters and cognitive function in children up to 12 years of age: Taiwanese maternal and infant cohort study. Gonzalez-Bulnes A, editor. PLoS One. Public Library of Science. 2015;10:e0131910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Kim JI, Hong Y-C, Shin CH, Lee YA, Lim Y-H, Kim B-N. The effects of maternal and children phthalate exposure on the neurocognitive function of 6-year-old children. Environ Res. 2017;156:519–25.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Kim Y, Ha E-H, Kim E-J, Park H, Ha M, Kim J-H, et al. Prenatal exposure to phthalates and infant development at 6 months: prospective Mothers and Children’s Environmental Health (MOCEH) study. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119:1495–500.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Doherty BT, Engel SM, Buckley JP, Silva MJ, Calafat AM, Wolff MS. Prenatal phthalate biomarker concentrations and performance on the Bayley scales of infant development-II in a population of young urban children. Environ Res. 2017;152:51–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Polanska K, Ligocka D, Sobala W, Hanke W. Phthalate exposure and child development: the polish mother and child cohort study. Early Hum Dev. 2014;90:477–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Whyatt RM, Liu X, Rauh VA, Calafat AM, Just AC, Hoepner L, et al. Maternal prenatal urinary phthalate metabolite concentrations and child mental, psychomotor, and behavioral development at 3 years of age. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;120:290–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Factor-Litvak P, Insel B, Calafat AM, Liu X, Perera F, Rauh VA, et al. Persistent associations between maternal prenatal exposure to phthalates on child IQ at age 7 years. Carpenter DO, editor. PLoS One. Public Library of Science. 2014;9:e114003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Heude B, Forhan A, Slama R, Douhaud L, Bedel S, Saurel-Cubizolles M-J, et al. Cohort profile: the EDEN mother-child cohort on the prenatal and early postnatal determinants of child health and development. Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45:353–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Chevrier C, Petit C, Philippat C, Mortamais M, Slama R, Rouget F, et al. Maternal urinary phthalates and phenols and male genital anomalies. Epidemiology. NIH Public Access. 2012;23:353–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Botton J, Philippat C, Calafat AM, Carles S, Charles M-A, Slama R, et al. Phthalate pregnancy exposure and male offspring growth from the intra-uterine period to five years of age. Environ Res. 2016;151:601–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Philippat C, Botton J, Calafat AM, Ye X, Charles M-A, Slama R. Prenatal exposure to phenols and growth in boys. Epidemiology. 2014;25:625–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Philippat C, Mortamais M, Chevrier C, Petit C, Calafat AM, Ye X, et al. Exposure to phthalates and phenols during pregnancy and offspring size at birth. Environ Health Perspect. 2012;120(3):464–70.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children: administration and scoring manual. 3rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Wechsler D. WPPS-IIII : échelle d’intelligence de Wechsler pour la période préscolaire et primaire. 3ème édition ed. Paris: ECPA; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Peyre H, Bernard JY, Hoertel N, Forhan A, Charles M-A, De Agostini M, et al. Differential effects of factors influencing cognitive development at the age of 5-to-6 years. Cogn Dev. 2016;40:152–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Ye X, Kuklenyik Z, Needham LL, Calafat AM. Measuring environmental phenols and chlorinated organic chemicals in breast milk using automated on-line column-switching-high performance liquid chromatography-isotope dilution tandem mass spectrometry. J Chromatogr B Analyt Technol Biomed Life Sci. 2006;831:110–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Silva M, Samandar E, Preaujr J, Reidy J, Needham L, Calafat A. Quantification of 22 phthalate metabolites in human urine. J Chromatogr B. 2007;860:106–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Mortamais M, Chevrier C, Philippat C, Petit C, Calafat AM, Ye X, et al. Correcting for the influence of sampling conditions on biomarkers of exposure to phenols and phthalates: a 2-step standardization method based on regression residuals. Environ Health. 2012;11:29.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med. 2011;30:377–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. O’Grady KE. A confirmatory maximum likelihood factor analysis of the WPPSI. Pers Individ Dif. 1990;11:135–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Stone BJ, Gridley BE, Gyurke JS. Confirmatory factor analysis of the WPPSI-R at the extreme end of the age range. J Psychoeduc Assess. Sage PublicationsSage CA: Thousand Oaks, CA. 1991;9:263–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing on JSTOR. J R Stat Soc. 1995;57:289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Sánchez BN, Budtz-Jørgensen E, Ryan LM, Hu H. Structural equation models. J Am Stat Assoc Taylor & Francis. 2005;100:1443–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Budtz-Jørgensen E, Keiding N, Grandjean P, Weihe P. Estimation of health effects of prenatal methylmercury exposure using structural equation models. Environ Health. 2002;1:2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Kline R. In: The Guilford press, editor. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3rd ed; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Calafat AM, Silva MJ, Reidy JA, Earl Gray L, Samandar E, Preau JL, et al. Mono-(3-Carboxypropyl) phthalate, a metabolite of Di- n -Octyl phthalate. J Toxicol Environ Health Part A. Taylor & Francis Group. 2006;69:215–27.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Téllez-Rojo MM, Cantoral A, Cantonwine DE, Schnaas L, Peterson K, Hu H, et al. Prenatal urinary phthalate metabolites levels and neurodevelopment in children at two and three years of age. Sci Total Environ. 2013;461–462:386–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Ali EHA, Elgoly AHM. Combined prenatal and postnatal butyl paraben exposure produces autism-like symptoms in offspring: comparison with valproic acid autistic model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2013;111:102–10.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Grandjean P, Landrigan PJ. Neurobehavioural effects of developmental toxicity. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13:330–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Robinson O, Basagaña X, Agier L, de Castro M, Hernandez-Ferrer C, Gonzalez JR, et al. The pregnancy exposome: multiple environmental exposures in the INMA-Sabadell birth cohort. Environ Sci Technol American Chemical Society. 2015;49:10632–41.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to; Lise Giorgis-Allemand and Anne Forhan for data management, the midwives at the Poitiers and Nancy obstetric centers (Lorraine Douhaud, Sophie Bedel, Brigitte Lortholary, Sophie Gabriel, Muriel Rogeon, and Monique Malinbaum) for data collection, the psychologists (Marie-Claire Cona and Marielle Paquinet), and the data entry operators (Patricia Lavoine, Josiane Sahuquillo and Ginette Debotte), and Antonia Calafat for technical assistance in measuring the urinary biomarkers. We are grateful to the EDEN participating families.

We also acknowledge the EDEN Mother-Child Cohort Study Group: M. A. Charles, M. de Agostini, A. Forhan, B. Heude, P. Ducimetière; M. Kaminski, M. J. Saurel-Cubizolles, P. Dargent-Molina, X. Fritel, B. Larroque, N. Lelong, L. Marchand, C. Nabet; I. Annesi-Maesano, R. Slama; V. Goua, G. Magnin, R. Hankard; O. Thiebaugeorges, M. Schweitzer, B. Foliguet, N. Job-Spira, JY. Bernard, B. de Lauzon-Guillain, A. Germa, F. Pierre, J. Lepeule and J.Botton.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from ANSES (grant EST-2010/2/126) and Fondation de France (grant 2015-00059545). The Eden cohort is supported by grants from FRM, Inserm, IReSP, Nestlé, French Ministry of health, ANR, Univ. Paris-Sud, InVS, ANSES and MGEN. Dorothy Nakiwala is supported by a doctoral grant from Université Grenoble Alpes.

Availability of data and materials

Please contact author for data requests.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Contributions

All authors discussed the results and implications and commented on the manuscript at all stages. DN conducted the main analyses and wrote the manuscript under the supervision of RS and CP. HP helped with the development of the SEM analysis. BH is the PI of the EDEN study and JB was involved in the development of the EDEN cohort protocol and the study follow up regarding cognitive development. RB helped with the critical revision of the manuscript. RS and CP were the overall advisors and provided technical insight and guidance. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dorothy Nakiwala.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants gave informed written consent for themselves and for their children to be included in the cohort.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Figure S1. Path diagram illustrating associations between phenols, phthalate metabolites and FSIQ of boys in the EDEN Cohort. Table S1. Estimated standardized factor loadings of outcome and exposure indicator variables. Table S2. Associations between the covariates included in our final SEM model and boys’ IQ. Table S3. Spearman correlation coefficients between biomarker concentrations measured in maternal urine (n = 452a). Table S4. Adjusted associations between phenols, phthalate metabolites and Full Scale IQ of boys at 5 years using SEM (N = 452). Table S5. Adjusted associations between phenols, phthalate metabolites and IQ (Manual based scales) of boys at 5 years in the EDEN cohort, using multiple linear regression (N = 452). Table S6. Adjusted associations between phenols, phthalate metabolites concentrations categorized in tertiles and IQ (Manual based scales) of boys at 5 years in the EDEN cohort, using multiple linear regression (N = 452). Table S7. Adjusted associations between phenols, phthalate metabolites and IQ of boys at 5 years in the EDEN cohort, stratified for center of recruitment. (DOCX 184 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nakiwala, D., Peyre, H., Heude, B. et al. In-utero exposure to phenols and phthalates and the intelligence quotient of boys at 5 years. Environ Health 17, 17 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12940-018-0359-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12940-018-0359-0

Keywords